Home What's New
Psychoanalytic Writings
Psychotherapy Service Email Forums and Groups
Process Press Links |
Robert M. Young Online Writings
ACROSS THE BORDERLINE
by Robert M. Young
I am here to sound a keynote not on behalf of any standing I
have in the clinical or theoretical domain of psychosis but on behalf of the
impulse which led a group of us from the Guild of Psychotherapists and Free
Associations to think it would be worthwhile to convene a conference on psychosis
which brought together psychiatrists, psychoanalysts, psychotherapists, various other
sorts of therapists and people who are concerned with that which we call the
psychotic. Our first impulse was a simple but regrettably unusual one the
desire to bring the psychotic into the same space where people who focus on
relationships, words, movements, roles, etc. live and work. I should add that our deeper
impulse was to show that it is already there and not dealt with and relegated
to institutions and drugs, by which I mean classical psychiatry. Our aim was and is to
promote dialogue and exploration of parts of human nature which are all too often
bracketed off and sequestered in the domain of the Other. I hasten to
acknowledge that they surely are other but will argue as I did at our
last conference that they are ubiquitous and not the inhabitants of some alien,
alienated realm which most of us never inhabit and which is tended by people sometimes
called alienists.
It has increasingly struck me as odd, inconsistent and even
hypocritical that the training of psychoanalysts and psychotherapists (except, as far as I
know, at the Arbours Association) carefully avoids work with psychotic patients. I am not
saying that the reasons for protecting students from psychotherapeutic work with florid
psychosis are insubstantial. I do feel, however, that this protectiveness is strange,
since some of the most important ideas in psychoanalysis and psychoanalytic psychotherapy
in recent decades ideas which trainees study assiduously have come directly
from pioneering analytic work with such patients. I am thinking of the work of Menlanie
Klein, D. W. Winnicott, Ronald Fairbairn, Wilfred Bion, Harold Searles, Otto Kernberg,
Herbert Rosenfeld, Margaret Little, Marion Milner, Hanna Segal, Donald Meltzer, Henri Rey,
Murray Jackson, John Steiner. I would add to this list the work of Francis Tustin, Sydney
Klein and Anne Alvarez on autistic phenomena and that of Lesley Sohn on the identificate
and Michael Sinason and Joscelyn Richards on the co-habitee. This work is not just good;
it is profound, moving, inspiring. It has led us to think of the psychotic as integral to
our humanity and has changed the range and level of many psychotherapists clinical
work. I am not here to review this work but to draw attention to certain features and
certain aspects of how we think about the psychotic in contrast with aspects of
traditional psychiatry and psychoanalysis. It could be said that in concentrating on the form
and structure of the unconscious and in using scientistic analogies drawn from
nineteenth and twentieth century physicalist physiological concepts, Freud and his
orthodox followers have left others to convey the content and the full emotional
resonance and impact of the psychotic in all of us.
Which brings me to my title. It took me the longest time to figure out
why the title of a Ry Cooder song felt right for this address. At one level the answer
lies in an obvious pun between the diagnostic category which most often brings a
psychotherapist into contact with material which is obviously psychotic I am struck
by the resonances betrween the diagnostic category borderline personality
disorder, on the one hand, and popular culture, on the other. But the more I thought
about it, the more I realised that I was thinking more generally and more ambitiously. I
apologise to anyone why has not had the good fortune to hear this song, either in
Cooders original rendition or the equally evocative cover version recently sung by
Willie Nelson. I have printed out the words and will play the song for anyone who would
like to hear it. Here they are:
Across the Borderline
by Ry Cooder, Jim Dickinson and John Hiatt
Theres a place where Ive been told
Every street is paved with gold
And its just across the borderline
And when its time to take your turn
Heres a lesson that you must learn
You can lose more than you ever hope to find.
When you reach that broken promised land
Where every dream slips through your hand
Then youll know that its too late to change your mind
Cause youve paid the price to come so far
Just to wind up where you are
And youre still just across the borderline
Up and down the Rio Grande
A thousand footprints in the sand
Breathe a secret no one can define
The river flows on like a breath
In between our life and death
(Tell me) Whos the next to cross the borderline?
But hope remains when pride is gone
And it keeps you movin on
Calling you across the borderline
And when you reach the broken promised land
Where every dream slips through your hand
Then youll know that its too late to change your mind
Cause youve paid the price to come this far
Just to wind up where you are
And youre still just across the borderline.
While ostensibly about Mexican wetbacks seeking to improve their lives
by getting across the border to America, it has many other resonances. On one side of
borderline things are definitely and clearly one way, and on the other side they are
expected to be very different, indeed. This is part of a longing of people in one
condition to be in a presumed opposite one a split between misery and the Promised
Land which (I hope you will not groan when I say) is of a paranoid-schizoid extremity. The
song also conjures up a vivid sense of the internal world, where we harbour splits between
the phantasy of the misery of an existing reality and the nirvana of a presumed better
place, a place which comes to mind when one hears the sound of A Train in the
Distance, since, as Paul Simon puts it in the song by that name, The thought
that life could be better is woven indelibly into our hearts and our brains. (I
listened to this song on my way to analysis for some years). If we could just get across
that fence, our longings could be fulfilled, but were stuck here stuck, as
John Steiner would have it, between the paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions, stuck
in a psychic retreat, a borderland. For those of you who may not be familiar with debates
about these matters, Steiners concept of pathological organisation is on offer as a
Kleinian characterisation of what more orthodox people call borderline personality
disorder (Alex Tarnopolsky has written a useful Rosetta Stone translation between the two,
in which he points out that only a quarter of British psychiatrists use the concept of
borderline personality disorder and half of those are analysts Tarnopolsky, 1992,
p. 78).
Some of the figures associated with the borderline vividly convey the
space I am referring to between what one experiences and what one hopes for a space
where it is all to easy to get side-tracked, so much so that we have made cultural heroes
of people with borderline personalities. One of the main points of my paper is to suggest
that we take this cultural fact seriously as we think about the place of the psychotic in
our everyday world. Our sense of certain actors and their typical roles merge
imperceptibly. Harry Dean Stanton is one such person. As if to help me make my point, he
sings a verse on the Cooder original, just as he acts as the balladeer in Cool Hand
Luke. In his most memorable role he portrayed the haunting, lost, abandoned,
bewildered everyman in Paris Texas and endless characters of a bemused and
alienated visage before and since. Other actors of this ilk are Harvey Keitel (Bad
Timing, Alice Doesnt Live Here Anymore, Blue Collar,
Reservoir Dogs, The Bad Lieutenant), Mickey Rourke (Angel
Heart), Nicholas Cage (Wild at Heart) and Sean Penn. The film, The
Border, for which Cooder wrote the music, stars another such iconic actor, perhaps
the quintessential one, Jack Nicholson, who most of us saw first as the hippie,
dope-smoking dropout lawyer in Easy Rider, along with two other figures of the
sort I am highlighting, Peter Fonda and Dennis Hopper, who has made a career of such parts
and lived the part, as well. According to last Sundays colour supplement Hopper felt
that the villain in David Lynchs Blue Velvet was precisely himself. He
has also portrayed a psychotic villain in Paris Trout.
Connoisseurs of Roger Corman B movies will have seen a lot
of Nicholson, including a wonderful scene where he leers with masochistic fulfilment when
he has had all his teeth pulled by a dentist in one sitting. I suppose he is the most
famous of all filmic borderline personalities. He is probably best remembered for his
portrayal of Randle McMurphy in One Flew Over the Cuckoos Nest, Ken
Keseys allegory on what we do to non-conformists whom we consider to be
irresponsible and a threat to the established authoritarian social and psychiatric order.
(Mention of Kesey points to the whole genre of borderline literature which
included his work, that of Kerouac and Tom Wolfes account of that subculture in The
Electric Kool Aid Acid Test. Tony Tanner has reflected more broadly on the genre in a
chapter entitled Edge City, i.e., the borderlands around the boundaries of
conventional society, which embraces Ken Kesey and others in fiction (Tanner, 1971, ch.
16). Returning to the cinema, there was a spate of films celebrating such people in the
wake of the sixties, notably Steelyard Blues, about a subculture of misfits,
centred on Donald Sutherland, Jane Fonda and Peter Boyle. Mike Lees
Naked is a recent example of a borderline hero. Jack Nicholson has played such
a person in many guises: the musician in Five Easy Pieces, the rustler in
Missouri Breaks (whose sidekick was Stanton), the used up astronaut in
Terms of Endearment, the criminal Joker in Batman, the Devil in
The Witches of Eastwick, and, finally and conveniently for my purpose, the
properly maniacal werewolf in Wolf, taking us to the full-blooded image of the
split person, the Jeckyll & Hyde of Robert Louis Stevenson and Michael Sinasons
concept of the cohabitee.
I will say something about the imagery of such notions, but I want to
start with the most extreme version. In his papers and workshops on the cohabitee, Sinason
maintains that mental illness is ubiquitous, that we are each and all a body with two (and
only two) minds. The second personality is not a repository of trauma. It is hard-wired
and cannot learn. It can accumulate experience and agglommerate thoughts but cannot think
or change in any way (Sinason, 1993, p. 214). He believes that the language of part and
whole objects is insufficient to convey the picture of humanity he holds that a
full-blooded other, a whole personality, cohabits with the I personality, with
all its range of abilities, feelings and irrationalities. We are not dealing here
with something madly Promethiam or Faustian like Baron von Frankenstein and his pitiful
creature, created by him but a separate being. The correct fictional renderings are
precisely Dr Jeckyll and Mr Hyde or the helpless man who turns into a werewolf in the
light of the full moon. Both are two minds in a single body, and this is Sinasons
sombre model of human nature. (I am told, by the way, that a Scottosh novel of the early
nineteeth-century, Confessions of a Justified Sinner, by James Hogg, has the same
Jekyll/Hyde theme, but I havent read it yet.)
I am very struck by the ubiquity and popularity of representations of
such an Other in popular culture. When I was a boy I had recurrent terrors in the darkness
and nightmares, evoked by a whole series of films about Frankenstein, his bride, Dracula,
the Mummy, the Invisible Man and the Wolf Man dozens of them, right up to
Abbott and Costello Meet Frankenstein in which, as I recall, all of them make
appearances. Ask me who is properly terrifying, and I will tell you of Boris Karloff, Bela
Lugosi and Lon Chaney Jr. These actors and the creatures they portrayed were far more real
to me than any boogie-man of Southern folk tales or any of the fiends in the ghost stories
told around the YMCA or Scout campfire. The whole range of them reappeared in Hammer Films
with Peter Cushing, Christopher Lee and Vincent Price and are the subjects of a revival
and remakes at the moment.
I find this a stark and dispiriting rendition of our humanity, which I
am having a tough time coming to terms with after doing my best to assimilate what I took
to be a version of human nature which was dispiriting enough, thank you very much. I mean,
of course, the equal billing of Eros and Thanatos in Civilization and Its Discontents and
in the writings of Melanie Klein, a veritable re-incarnation of Manicheanism, a
religion which vied with the (believe it or not) more hopeful doctrine of Christianity, in
which man carries the birthright of the slaying of Abel by Cain in the doctrine original
sin, whereby we are literally born in sin and, like Bunyans Pilgrim, have to work
our salvation in a life of good works and repentance. In Christianity the ultimate triumph
of the good is assured. In Manicheanism, the forces of good and evil were equally matched,
and the overall outcome was very much in doubt and hung in the balance. Mind you, perhaps
Kleinianism strikes me, relatively speaking, as a form of optimism, since I was brought up
a strict Presbyterian, wherein the elect were already chosen, predestined, but
one had to behave well nevertheless. I later came to see this as rather like B. F.
Skinners intermittent reinforcement regime in which the rat got driven mad by the
lack of any connection between what it did and the rewards which came its way. It is not
surprising that Christianity won out over Manicheanism and that most psychoanalysts and
psychotherapists find undiluted Kleinianism and, I should acknowledge, some
undiluted Kleinians - too much to bear.
But there is also a compassionate and forgiving way of speaking about
these things. In doing so, I want to return to the concept of a border, borderlines or
borderlands It strikes me that although traditional psychiatry and traditional
psychoanalysis give plenty of weight to the irrational, they are very keen to draw a line.
This is clear in the whole tenor of the American Psychiatric Associations Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual (which, by the way, many of the psychotherapeutic trainees I
teach have never seen unless they have worked in mental hospitals). As Robert Wallerstein
points out in his review of the recent IPA research conference on Borderline Personality
Disorder, the diagnostic criteria used in DSM were quite consciously designed to be
atheoretical and behaviourally-based (Wallerstein, 1994, p. 765).
They are avowedly undynamic. Indeed, one makes the diagnosis on the basis of five
any five of eight criteria. The rubric reads, A pervasive pattern of
instability of mood, interpersonal relationships, and self-image, beginning in early
adulthood and present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by at least five of
the following: (Ill read the rest even though it will be familiar to many,
because I want to draw attention to how undynamic a list it is.)
(1) a pattern of unstable and intense interpersonal relationships
characterized by alternating between extremes of overidealization and devaluation
(2) impulsiveness in at least two areas that are potentially
self-damaging, e. g., spending, sex, substance abuse, shoplifting, reckless driving, binge
eating (Do not include suicidal or self-mutilating behavior covered in [5].)
(3) affective instability: marked shifts from baseline mood to
depression, irritability, or anxiety, usually lasting a few hours and only rarely more
than a few days
(4) inappropriate, intense anger or lack of control of anger, e.
g., frequent displays of temper, constant anger, recurrent physical fights
(5) recurrent suicidal threats, gestures, or behavior, or
self-mutilating behavior
(6) marked and persistent identity disturbance manifested by
uncertainty about at least two of the following: self-image, sexual orientation, long-term
goals or career choice, type of friends desired, preferred values
(7) chronic feelings of emptiness or boredom
(8) frantic efforts to avoid real or imagined abandonment (Do not
include suicidal or self-mutilating behavior covered in [5}.) (APA, 1987, p. 347).
There can also be transient psychotic symptoms at times of stress (p.
346). Wallerstein notes that this gives ninety-three combinations which can land one in
the BPD pigeon-hole (p. 675) or, to follow my own imagery, on the wrong side of the
borderline. It is a list calculated to make most people I know pretty uncomfortable.
I have spelled all this out in order to highlight a contrast on which
Wallerstein dwells. He refers to a clash of underlying philosophical assumptions
between diagnostic understandings derived from DSM-III criteria and those derived from
clinical psychodynamic formulations. What DSM-III rests on in its several hundred
categorisations and sub-categorisations is what all the individuals assigned to a
particular diagnostic pigeonhole have in common, their common behavioural
diagnostic defined class, for example, borderline personality disorders. What
psychoanalytic case formulation rests on is the understanding of a unique life
history eventuating in the presenting illness picture: what makes this particular
individual a unique instance, different from every other individual on earth. This focus
on uniqueness is shared, of course, with imaginative literature... (p. 766).
This brings me to several dichotomies, some false. First, I want to
distinguish between the sort of diagnostic pigeon-holing which characterises DSM-III and
classical psychiatry, from the sorts of dynamic formulations which characterise
psychoanalysis and related approaches. Medicine and, within it, psychiatry strives to find
instances of disease entities which are as near to the natural kinds of natural science as
can be managed. The model is the element in chemistry or the species in biology. However,
disease syndromes are not natural kinds. They are a coming together of a series of
phenomena which lead to an outcome which distresses people or those around them. Even
somatic diseases fail to qualify as natural kinds. It only takes a moments
reflection to see that elements such as iron or sulphur, or a species of beetle are very
different from the coming together of a micro-organism and a human to produce measles, a
genetic defect leading to a failure of insulin productionl leading to diabetes, various
problems in the circulatory system leading to a stroke or heart attack, and that these, in
turn, are very different from categories in DSM-II such as factitious disorder,
kleptomania, fetishism, trichotillomania (obsessively twizzling ones hair) or
self-defeating personality disorder. It is for this reason that psychiatric diagnosis is
so controversial. Syndromes come and go in successive editions of the manual, as
homosexuality did. It was introduced in 1953 and removed in 1973. Many of the diagnostic
categories spelled out toward the back of DSM-III are pretty dodgy, and some depict people
who are merely sad or obnoxious. Others fetishists and some sexual deviants, for
example are campaigning to get the stigma of having a psychiatric pigeonhole
removed from them. Nymphomaniacs and priapists were incarcerated when I was first a
psychiatric aide in 1955; they are roaming the streets now. But, then, so are people most
of us would agree need more institutional care than is currently available.
I will add here that there is an intriguing literature on the
historical relativity of somatic medical diagnoses which lends extra weight to my
scepticism about the categories of psychiatric psychopathology, since it makes the
classsifications of the parent discipline, medicine, less apparently biological and more
conventional and cultural. A series of searching papers by Karl Figlio has made this point
vividly with respect to chlorosis (a form of anemia) and miners nystagmus (Figlio,
1978, 1979, 1985). The eminent psychiatrist and historian of medicine German Berrios
points out that the putative objectivity and neutrality of psychiatric classifications is
a will-o-the wisp, relying on the empiricist tradition for its respectability but in fact
begging large questions about the theory-ladenness and value-ladenness of its concepts
(Berrios, 1991, esp. p. 236). Indeed, he makes the point forcefully in saying that the
persistence of psychopathological classifications in British psychiatry can be attributed
to an empiricist false-consciousness, seeking theory-neutral descriptions. Its basis
the wish to remain at the level of description is an effort to be free of
the obligation to understand and deal with process, dynamics and aetiology. He also points
out that the reliability coefficients of psychiatric diagnoses are not very impressive (p.
241) Borderline Personality Disorder is a striking example of a diagnosis which has been
the subject of intense controversy, culminating in a recent, good collection on the
validity of the concept (Silver and Rosenbleuth, 1992)
Even the most oft-cited criteria for diagnosing the most
well-established psychiatric syndromes are not the exclusive domain of, say, the
schizophrenic, the manic-depressive or the so-called true paranoid. I am referring to
hallucinations and delusions, the two mainstays of the distinction between normal and
psychotic. It could be said that the distinguishing feature of the psychoanalytic
tradition which runs from Abraham and Klein through Bion, Segal, Meltzer and Rosenfeld is
the stress it lays on the presence of psychotic phenomena in all of us. In her classical
paper On the Genesis of Psychical Conflict in Early Infancy (which was one of
the main documents in the Freud-Klein Controversial Discussions see King and
Steiner, 1991), Joan Riviere bases her claims about the ubiquity of psychotic processes in
infants on Freuds own hypothesis that the psyche is always interpreting the
reality of its experiences 'or rather, misinterpreting them in a
subjective manner that increases its pleasure and preserves it from pain' (Riviere, 1952a,
p. 41). Freud calls this process "hallucination"; and it forms the foundation of
what we mean by phantasy-life'. Riviere adds that 'this primitive and elementary function
of his psyche to misinterpret his perceptions for his own satisfaction still
retains the upper hand in the minds of the great majority of even civilised adults' (p.
41). Klein notoriously and repeatedly said that the thought that primitive mental
processes of infants were like those of adult psychotics. She denied that this was
tantamount to saying that they were psychotic, but her loyal disciple, Donald Meltzer,
says it is difficult to draw any other conclusion. He says the same thing about
Bions distinction between the psychotic and the non-psychotic parts of the
personality. Some said that this distinction was only being made about the minds of
schizophrenics, but Meltzer comes down firmly on the side of saying that Bion means all
personalities, especially including those of analytic trainees.
The penchant of Kleinians for finding psychotic processes in all of us
is striking. Klein moved from locating the paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions as
the starting points for adult psychoses to treating them as developmental stages and
finally to seeing them as present in the moment-to-moment shifts in peoples thought
processes. John Steiner says of those positions and his own concept of the borderline
position, stuck between the two which he calls a pathological organization or
psychic retreat that It is clear that not only the two basic
positions but also the borderline position occur in all patients, and the notion of
positions can help the analyst to consider where the patient is located at any particular
time (Steiner, 1993, p. 11). His work is replete with examples of the ubiquity and
normality of such processes (see, e.g., pp. 26, 51, 52, 54). The person who puts this
point best and most frequently and to whose work I shall revert in a moment is Harold
Searles, who says, I became convinced, long ago, that borderline phenomena will be
encountered in any deep-reaching course of psychoanalysis or intensive psychoanalytic
therapy, for these phenomena are part of the general human condition (Searles, 1986,
p. xii).
But classical Freudian psychoanalysis tells a different story, a story
of well-drawn and well-guarded borderlines. It is, I think, for this reason that Searles
says of orthodox psychoanalysis, 'to the degree that it is rigorously classical, it is
delusional' (Searles, 1979, p. 458). It has a model based on keeping the irrational at
bay. I can best illustrate this by drawing your attention to two psychoanalytic classics,
Freuds The Psychopathology of Everyday Life (1901) and Anna Freuds The
Ego and the Mechanisms of Defence (1936). I want to stress that in drawing attention
to the affinities between these two works I am distorting the richness of Sigmund
Freuds work, teasing out a particular strand and emphasising how it got woven into
what came to be known as neo-Freudian orthodoxy, a reading of his work which until
recently dominated most of the psychoanalytic world.
The model is this. The ego is the guardian of realism and adaptation.
It has at its disposal a set of mechanisms which are like mini-neuroses, which work like
safety valves, letting off a bit of steam in a parapraxis, a slip of the tongue, a
forgotten letter or stamp or name or signature or appointment, a momentary clumsiness.
This restores the equilibrium in the same way dreams do. Indeed, he points out the
similarities between parapraxes, on the one hand, and dreams and full-blown neurotic
symptoms, on the other (Freud, 1901, pp. 277-8). He concludes that in parapraxes the
symptoms are located in the least important psychical functions, while everything that can
lay claim to higher psychical value remains free from disturbance (p. 279).
Anna Freud sought to classify the mechanisms available to the ego to
achieve a kind of psychic equilibrium. I my view she lumps together mechanisms of
tremendously different levels of significance. projection, introjection, repression,
sublimation, and conversion strike me as basic to the deepest levels of mental
functioning, while others, e. g., undoing, isolation of affect and turning against the
self, are on a different level, and still others such as denial and regression fall
somewhere in between. In a fascinating book-length series of interviews with her in which
this classic is re-evaluated near the end of Anna Freuds life, Joseph Sandler makes
it clear that the list is far from definitive or complete (Sandler, 1985). Anna Freud
explored about thirteen; I was taught nineteen as a medical student. The main point of her
conceptualisation of the egos mechanisms of defence is to keep irrationalities from
erupting into consciousness and to maximise the conflict-free sphere of ego functioning.
This approach depends on a model of the mind where there is a border
between the rational and irrational, between ego and id, which is policed by the
egos platoon of keepers of the peace. How different a conception of the inner world
this conveys from one in which we are shunting back and forth between splitting,
projective identification and persecution, on the one hand, and integration, depressive
anxiety and reparation, on the other. Notice that both of the Kleinian positions include
terms which are familiar to the psychiatry of psychosis. Indeed, as I have indicated, they
had their origins in her investigations in the 1930s into the origins of manic-depressive
and paranoid psychoses. Hence: paranoid, schizoid and depressive in the Kleinian concepts
which provide the most basic and common features of our everyday thinking. In a classical
neo-Freudian model, the irrational goes on one side of the line and the rational on the
other. In the Kleinian model they are jumbled, and we are constantly shuffling between
these two basic positions, managing paired emotions love and hate, envy and
gratitude with constant difficulty, living much of the time near the edge or,
perhaps I can say, in the borderlands.
In case you are wondering where we have been and are going, Ill
put up some signposts. I have been reflecting on classification and on the line drawn in
psychiatry and orthodox psychoanalysis between the normal and the psychotic. I have been
contrasting that frame of mind with some things we experience in our everyday and cultural
lives, drawing on a song and some movies, notably ones involving people who get labelled
as having borderline personalities when they get incarcerated but who evoke lots of
identifications when we see them on the screen and the gossip columns. I have also pointed
out the close fit between Mike Sinasons extreme view of the place of madness in us
with another genre of films in which two identities inhabit a single body. My overall aim
is to bring the Otherness of psychosis nearer to the rest of life and to draw attention to
the ubiquitousness of psychotic phenomena in our development, our inner worlds and our
culture in a quite mundane, day-by day and minute-by-minute sense.
Now I want to go onto the opposite tack, because I would be horrified
if you thought I was saying that there is no such thing as psychosis or that treating
borderline or psychotic people was just like treating anyone else. Having brought the
psychotic into everyday life I now want to leave open the question of how we distinguish
properly psychotic people from the psychotic parts and themes and moments in the rest of
us. My purpose today is to throw doubt on the practice of drawing lines. How we go on to
characterise psychotically disturbed people is a problem I suggest we address after we are
clear about that. I would rather, for example, speak in terms of relative refractoriness
or the extent to which the psychotic is in the ascendant in a given person at a given
time.
First, I need hardly say in this company that working
psychotherapeutically with psychotic and borderline people is hard, exhausting, demanding,
dispiriting and has to be approached with a species of stoicism and with limited goals.
All writers make this clear, but Harold Searles makes it clearest of all. He is eloquent
in conveying the impact of their projections, the problems of maintaining the transference
and, most challenging of all, how excruciatingly tough it is to bear what the patient
evokes in the countertransference. I have a psychotic patient who has often been
hospitalised and came to me after wearing out a colleague and who regales me in a
stentorian voice, always in an expository mode, with long sagas from his multigenerational
delusional system, with whom I find it all too easy to let my mind wander and who makes
little or no response to most interpretations, Id say half to others and
seems for the most part unable to make any conscious connection with transference
interpretations. He is so sensitive to others projections that he cannot bear drama
on television or in the theatre and always feels potentially overwhelmed, just as he was
by his parents. Yet he has improved and holds down a modest job in the helping
professions, has maintained a marriage and functions (albeit with lots of jealousy) as a
parent.
I have two patients who have pathological organisations who I have seen
for nearly eight years each. I would have to say, with some embarrassment, that one has
not shifted fundamentally, though I think I can discern signs of movement at the moment.
Her reigning symptom is being stuck in her cosy relationship, sexually and in
career terms, and the mood of our sessions is one of passivity, filled with what Betty
Joseph aptly calls chuntering, a constant flow without purchase. She suffers
from a kind of wistful longing; everything its muted; nothing will change; she does not
expect anything. She has a perpetual sense unconnected to any concrete plans
that if she could only return to her native country she could live again and had a vivid
dream of gazing across a narrow isthmus to a mountain in the distance where all would be
well if she could only reach it. She has little or no conscious sense of the perverseness
and the destructive narcissism controlling much of her inner world and engendering her
severe self-limitation. Yet she perseveres. She once tried to terminate treatment but
reacted with such distress that she was back within days. In both of these cases my main
task is to avoid collusive patterns of relating where I am the only person in the room
interested in change.
My other patient with a pathological organisation is the most
challenging person with whom I have worked. Without the supervision and support of Alex
Tarnopolsky and Arthur Hyatt Williams, I dont know what I might have done. She works
in a cultural job and is conventionally successful, if by that you mean getting work and
having it well-received and widely experienced. But she hates it, hates most of life and
despised the work. After many years of saying she wanted to but couldnt she did
manage to do a course and, amid continual claims that nothing mattered or gave any
satisfaction, finished it with distinction and was soon tapped for the countrys
premier position in her field. Of course, this turns out to be meaningless, too. So much
for half of Freuds goal to love and to work. On the love side she has never
made love; she has only had sex to hurt people and not even that for five years. She
envies people in relationships, hates the sun and the spring and greenery and cannot bear
it when people have babies. She experiences a perverse delight in despising the good: fair
is foul and foul is fair. She acknowledges no relationship with me, does not explicitly
grant the pertinence of interpretations (though she often confirms them unconsciously),
has no gratitude and cannot hold onto a good experience for a moment after the actual
event. And just as the DSM rubric says she has transient psychotic symptoms
from time to time. She believes that black objects will attack her, that black bags full
of offal will be thrown through her car window, that flexes and garden hoses will strangle
her and that black men will leap out of the attic and kill her. She once hallucinated a
puddle of water in her bedroom which disappeared before her eyes. Yet she works regularly,
is admired in her profession, comes regularly to her sessions, makes an effort to make up
ones lost through work and conveys again, unconsciously a longing and need
for containment unequalled in my clinical experience. She often says that if she could
only move to Spain or be a waitress or receptionist, life would be bearable across the
borderline from where she is at.
I have spoken about these patients to make it clear that I have some
idea about the intractability of psychotic and borderline phenomena. But, like Searles, I
believe passionately in this work and find it rewarding in a way which cannot easily be
characterised. He stresses the need to acknowledge that what the patient puts into one
finds a home and evokes distressing feelings of worthlessness, of being non-human and of
sheer ennui, with boundaries between the patients inner world and ones
own hard to find or maintain, while one has the concomitant responsibility to contain
I want to say to contain like mad. The patient I have just been
discussing is quite literally hopeless, and I am the repository of good experiences, hope
and quite a lot of memory.
Id like to share a couple of passages from Searles which convey
the special character of this work. He quotes a patient: I dont care anything
about the way you respond. I care about how I respond. Your feelings
dont mean anything more to me than if you were one of the lines on that wallpaper
there. He comments that it took him years to discern that for such patients
the therapist is in actuality of such basic importance that the patient cannot allow
more than a little bit of the therapist to be perceived as being outside the
patient (Searles, 1986, pp. 31-2.). In a later passage he expands on the problem of
the borderline patients inability to differentiate at a more than superficial
level, between nocturnal dreams or daytime fantasies on the one hand, and perception of
outer reality on the other hand; between thoughts (and/or feelings) and behavioral
actions; between symbolic and concrete levels of meaning in communications; between
himself and the other person; between himself and the outer world; between human and
nonhuman, animate and inanimate, ingredients of the outer world; and so on (p. 58).
I now want to revert to Wallersteins distinction between what
patients have in common and what is unique about them. This may seem a rather bland
distinction, but I think it parallels what makes psychotherapists feel uncomfortable about
conventional psychiatry, psychiatric nosology or classification and psychiatric
institutions. When I first worked in a mental hospital in the mid-1950s, I felt a real
split. When I attended diagnostic interviews and classes I became very adept at making
diagnoses according to the official rubric. I was an apt pupil. This was the precise
moment when utterly custodial psychiatry was being challenged by the new major
tranquillizers, with the effect that people were again beginning to be seen as
individuals, and the issue of unlocking most of the wards was coming onto the agenda. When
I went out onto the ward, however, my experience of the patients was truly as individuals,
but I had the strong impression that if I had stayed there as long as some of the staff I
would have lost that sense of individuality and fallen back on perceiving the patient as
the diagnosis.
Once again, this is a common feature of socialisation into the parent
discipline, medicine. In her classic study of what happens to nurses during training,
Isabel Menzies Lyth points out that a major feature of the reification that occurs leads
one to refer to the liver in bed 10 or the pneumonia in bed 15,
thereby depersonalising the patient (Lyth, 1959, p. 52). She analyses this in terms of
being socialised into a system of defences against psychotic anxieties which protect the
staff from potentially overwhelming primitive feelings associated with life-threatening
situations, feelings which re-evoke infantile anxieties. This way of thinking has been
applied to all groups and institutions by Bion, Elliott Jaques, A. K. Rice, Pierre
Turquet, Eric Miller, Gordon Lawrence, David Armstong, Bob Hinshelwood and others in the
so-called group relations movement (Bion, 1961; Jaques, 1955; Turquet, 1975;
Miller, 1990; Lawrence, 1991; Armstrong, 1991, 1992). It is hells own job to remain
human in such settings, and Donald Meltzer (1992) has given us a vivid description of the
sort of person who rises to the top of them, living in their inner world at the other
extreme, the claustrum, the lower end of the psychic digestive tract, desperately
defending themselves against schizophrenic breakdown and doing so at the expense of
sensitive and thoughtful human relations.
But it can be done, as Searles and others have shown. I am thinking of
Searles analytic work with psychotics and borderlines, sustained at Chesnut Lodge
and elsewhere over many decades, and Peter Barhams sensitive renderings of the
utterances of a full-blown schizophrenic which may appear incomprehensible on the surface
but make good sense when properly understood in the context of his life and environment
(Barham, 1985, esp. ch. 4). In this context we should also acknowledge the pioneering work
of Ronald Laing and Joseph Berke in insisting that we attend fully and patiently to what
psychotic people say and mean. What I am suggesting is that, although there are strong
unconscious, disciplinary and institutional forces at work which can lead us
all-too-easily to draw a sharp borderline between the normal and the psychotic or Other,
it really is possible to hold onto the narrative of the individual life and its inner
meaning, as it interacts at the deepest level with the inner world of the therapist and
other people. It is also possible to conceptualise the primitive processes which we
characterise as psychotic, without falling into the well of jaded categorisation which
stresses the common features of disturbed thought at the expense even to the near
exclusion of the individual life stories and idiosyncratic meanings which
constitute our humanity. Barhams writings have been as eloquent as those of Searles
in holding onto the meaningfulness of the psychotic without settling for the reifications
of diagnostic nosology.
I think the future of relations between dynamic psychotherapy and
dynamic psychiatry lies in a shared project of holding onto that individual narrative
meaningfulness without flinching at just how distressing and testing it is for the mental
health worker. As I think of the art of Bosch and Breughel and van Gogh and Dali and Man
Ray, the fiction of John Barth and Joseph Heller especially the characters of Milo
Minderbinder and Yossarian and recall some of the film actors and characters I
mentioned earlier, I know that that shared project would bring us into the mainstream of
what the wider culture has always known about the psychotic, something
conveyed in a saying first coined by Terence in the second century BC and worth recalling
and striving to hold onto in our daily psychiatric and psychotherapeutic work:
Nothing human is alien to me.
This is the text of the keynote address to the Second International Conference on
Psychosis: Treatment of Choice?, at the University of Essex, Colchester, 23-25
September 1994.
REFERENCES
(Place of Publication is London unlkess otrherwise specified.)
Abbott and Costello Meet Frankenstein
Alvarez, Anne (1992) Live Company: Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy with Autistic,
Borderline, Deprived and Abused Children. Routledge.
APA (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Edition -
Revised). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. (DSM or DSM-III).
Armstrong, David (1991) 'The "Institution in the Mind":
Reflections on the Relation of Psycho-analysis to Work with Institutions', paper read to
the conference on Psychoanalysis and the Public Sphere, Polytechnic of East London, 1-2
November.
______ (1992) Names, Thoughts and Lies: the Relevance of
Bions Later Writings for Understanding Experiences in Groups, Free Assns. (no.
26) 3: 261-82.
Barham, Peter (1984) Schizophrenia and Human Value. Oxford:
Blackwell; reprinted Free Association Books, 1993.
Batman
Berrios, German E. (1991) British Psychopathology since the Early
Twentieth Century, in G. E. Berrios and Hugh Freeman, eds., 150 Years of British
Psychiatry, 1841-1991. Gaskell, pp. 232-44.
Bion, W R. (1955) 'Group Dynamics - a Re-view', in Klein et al., eds.
(1955), pp. 440-77.
______(1961) Experiences in Groups and Other Papers. Tavistock.
Blue Velvet
Borderline
Cooder, Ry (199?) Across the Borderline
Easy Rider
Fairbairn, W. R. D. (1952)
Figlio, Karl (1978) "Chlorosis and Chronic Disease in Nineteenth-century Britain:
The Social Constitution of Somatic Illness in a Capitalist Society, Internat. J.
Health Services 8: 589-617.
______ (1979) Sinister Medicine: A Critique of Left Approaches to Medicine,
Rad. Sci. J. 9: 14-68.
______ (1985) Medical Diagnosis, VClass Dynamics, Social Stability, in L.
Levidow and R. M. Young, eds., Science, Technology and the Labour ~Process: Marxist
Studies. Free Association Books, vol. 2, pp. 129-65.
Five Easy Pieces
Freud, Anna (1936) The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defence. Hogarth.
Freud, Sigmund (1901?) The Psychoathology of Everyday Life
______ (1930) Civilization and Its Discontents
Hinshelwood, R. D. (1987) What Happens in Groups. Free
Association Books.
______(1991) A Dictionary of Kleinian Thought, 2nd ed. Free
Association Books.
Jackson, Murray and Williams, Paul (1994) Unimaginable Storms:
The Search for Meaning in Psychosis. Karnac.
Jaques, E. (1951) The Changing Culture of a Factory: A Study of
Authority and Participation in an Industrial Setting. Routledge & Kegan Paul.
______(1955) 'Social Systems as a Defence against Persecutory and
Depressive Anxiety', in Klein et al., eds. (1955), pp. 478-98.
Kernberg, Otto (1975) Borderline Conditions and Pathological Narcissism. N.Y.:
Aronson.
Kesey, Ken (1962) One Flew Over the Cuckoos Nest. N. Y.: Viking
King, Pearl and Steiner, Ricardo, eds. (1991) The Freud-Klein Controversies 1941-45. Tavistock/Routledge.
Klein, Melanie et al. (1952) Developments in Psychoanalysis. Hogarth;
reprinted Karnac, 1989.
______, eds (1955) New Directions in Psycho-Analysis: The
Significance of Infant Conflicts in the Patterns of Adult Behaviour. Tavistock;
reprinted Maresfield, 1977.
Klein, Sydney (1980) Autistic Phenomena in Neurotic
Patients, Internat. J. Psycho-anal. 61: 395-402.
Lawrence, W. Gordon (1991) Won from the Void of the Infinite:
Experiences of Social Dreaming, Free Assns. (no. 22) 2: 259-94.
Little, Margaret (1986) Transference Neurosis and Transference
Psychosis: Toward Basic Unity. Aronson; reprinted Free Association Books/Maresfield.
Lyth, Isabel Menzies (1959) 'The Functions of Social Systems as a
Defence Against Anxiety: A Report on a Study of the Nursing Service of a General
Hospital', Human Relations 13:95-121; reprinted in Containing Anxiety in
Institutions: Selected Essays, vol. 1. Free Association Books, pp. 43-88.
McClellan, Jim (1994) Speed Freak, Life: The Observer
Magazine. 18 September, pp. 20-26.
Meltzer. D. (1966) 'The Relation of Anal Masturbation to Projective
Identification', Int. J. Psycho-anal,. 47:335-42; reprinted in Spillius (1988),
vol. 1, pp 102-16.
______(1992) The Claustrum: An Investigation of Claustrophobic
Phenomena. Strath Tay: Clunie.
______ et al. (1975) Explorations in Autism: A
Psycho-Analytical Study. Strath Tay: Clunie.
Miller, Eric (1990) 'Experiential Learning Groups I: The Development of
the Leicester Model', in E. Trist and H. Murray, eds., The Social Engagement of Social
Science: A Tavistock Anthology, Vol. 1: The Socio-Psychological Perspective. Free
Association Books, pp. 165-85.
Missouri Breaks
Nelson, Willie (1993) Across the Borderline
One Flew Over the Cuckoos Nest
Richards, Joscelyn (1993) Cohabitation and the Negative Therapeutic
Reaction, Psychoanal. Psychother. 7:223-39.
Rey, Henri (1994) Universals of Psychoanalysis: in the Treatment of Psychotic and
Borderline States. Free Association Books.
Rice, A. K. (1965) Learning for Leadership. Tavistock.
Riviere, Joan (1952) General Introduction, in Klein et al. (1952),
pp. 1-36.
______ (1952a) On the Genesis of Psychical Conflict in Early Infancy, in
Klein et al. (1952), pp. 37-66.
Rosenfeld, H. (1969) On the Treatment of Psychotic States by
Psychoanalysis: An Historical Approach, Internat. J. Psycho-anal. 50:615-31
______ (1971) 'A Clinical Approach to the Psychoanalytic Theory of the
Life and Death Instincts: An Investigation into the Aggressive Aspects of Narcissism', Int.
J. Psycho-anal. 52:169-78; reprinted in Spillius (1988), vol. 1, pp. 239-55.
Sandler, Joseph with Anna Freud (1985) The Analysis of Defense: The Ego and the
Mechanisms of Defense Revisited. N. Y.: International Universities Press.
Searles, Harold (1979) Countertransference and Related Subjects.
N. Y: International Universities Press.
______ (1986) My Work with Borderline Patients. Aronson
Silver, Daniel and Rosenbleuth, Michael, eds. (1992) Handbook of
Borderline Disorders. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Simon, Paul (198?) Train in the Distance
Sinason, Michael (1993) Who Is the Mad Voice Inside?, Psyhchoanal.
Psychother. 7: 207-21.
Sohn, Leslie (1985?) Anorexic and Bulemic States of Mind in the
Psycho-analytic Treatment of Anorexic/Biulemic Patients and Psychotic Patients, Psychoanal.
Psychother. 1: 49-56.
______ (1985) Narcissistic Organization, Projective
Identification and the Formation of the Identificate, Internat. J. Psycho-anal
66:201-13; reprinted in E. Spillius, ed. (1988), vol. 1, pp. 271-92.
Spillius, E. B.. (1988) Melanie Klein Today, 2 vols. Routledge
Steelyard Blues
Steiner, John (1993) Psychic Retreats. Routledge
Stevenson, Robert Louis (1886) The Strange Case of Dr Jekyll and Mr
Hyde and Other Stories; reprinted Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1979.).
Tanner, Tony (1971) Edge City, in City of Words:
American Fiction1950-1970. Harper & Row, pp. 372-91.
Tarnopolsky, Alex (1992) Borderline Disoprdrs: A British Point of
View, in D. Silver and M. Rosenbleuth, eds, 1992, pp. 177-202.
Turquet, Pierre (1975) Threats to Identity in the Large Group, in L.
Kreeger, Lionel, ed., The Large Group: Dynamics and Therapy. Constable; reprinted
Maresfield.
Tustin, Francis (1986) Autistic Barriers in Neurotic Patients. Karnac.
Winnicott, D. W. (19?? Hate in the Countertransference,
The Witches of Eastwick
Wolf
Wallerstein, Robert (1994) Borderline Disorders: Report on the
4th IPA Research Conference, Internat. J. Psycho-anal. 75: 763-74.
Wolfe, Tom (19??) The Electric Kool Aid Acid Test
Young, Robert M. (1994) Mental Space. Process Press.
Address for correspondence: 26 Freegrove Road, London N7 9RQ
tel 071 607 8306 fax 071 609 4837
robert@rmy1.demon.co.uk
© The Author
|
|