THE HOUSE OF TRAUMA
by Robert M. Young
In the house of trauma there are many mansions.
I have been asked to strike a keynote, and it is contained in
that sentence. In this short introduction I can only indicate some of them and
hope I can intrigue you into returning to them and the broader issues about
diagnostic categories which they raise (see, e.g., Young 1998, 1998a, 1999).
Trauma conjures up many meanings, extending from a humble term loosely conveying
the harm, the injury that leads people to be troubled and seek psychotherapy or
counselling, through a complex history of ideas including successively:
hysteria, shell shock, war neurosis to the currently fashionable concept of
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, the last of which strikes me as making a
hegemonic bid in psychopathology which is parallel in some ways to the bid that
the discipline of Cultural Studies made some years ago to take over all the
arts.
Here are some data:
There are currently 601 books listed under trauma at
Amazon.co.uk and 1215 for Amazon US.
The traumatic-stress forum on the internet (T-S) has 484
subscribers. Its moderator, Professor Charles Figley, runs a major centre in
Florida, one of many in the US and here, for example, there is one in
Nottingham. He is the author of sixteen books and innumerable articles and
founded the main journal Traumatology. The T-S Forum focuses on the all matters
of interest to the emerging field of Traumatology, which is the study,
treatment, and reporting of information about the immediate and long-term
psychosocial, and psychobiological consequences of highly stressful events and
circumstances. Forum members are researchers, practitioners, reporters, policy
makers, students, professors, and others who are interested in contributing to
the field. There is a world-wide Green Cross network of experts available to
respond to disasters.
At a well-attended conference at Sheffield University
recently decorated briefcases were handed out and there was a session on the
traumatic stress suffered by those who attend to traumatized people. One of the
main topics was ‘compassion fatigue’.
A trilogy on war neurosis by Pat Barker (1991, 1993, 1995)
was a recent best seller. One volume, The Ghost Road, won the Booker Prize, and
another, Regeneration, was later made into a film. At the heart of the trilogy
was the work of a psychoanalyst and anthropologist W. H. R. Rivers, who was
working compassionately with shell-shocked soldiers. Similar work was done by
Wilfred Bion and a number of other founders of the group relations and
therapeutic community movement, especially in the Northfield experiment in the
Second World War (Harrison, 2000).
If, as we should, we cast the net more widely we come upon
the designation hysteria and shell-shock from the nineteenth and early twentieth
centuries, the former with its Boswell in Elaine Showalter (1985) and the latter
with a truly admirable history by Allan Young (1995) which you should read if
you haven’t already. It is a masterpiece on the historicity and the social
construction of diagnostic categories.
You will probably know that the term has been used very
widely, indeed. The OED concentrates on ‘wound’, ‘injury’, ‘abrasion’ but
includes ‘morbid nervous condition’. The social and intellectual historian and
biographer of Freud, Peter Gay, wrote of the rapid changes in the eighteenth and
nineteenth centuries. ‘You cannot have serious change without trauma’, while
diagnosticians of battle fatigues or shell-shock originally conjured up a
literal impact from exploding ordinance, only to notice fairly rapidly that the
same symptoms were exhibited by people who were not anywhere near artillery
bombardment or any impact from such physical shock waves. My computer offers
those two meanings: (1) an extremely distressing experience that causes severe
emotional shock and may have long-lasting psychological effects; (2) a physical
injury or would to the body. In somatic medicine trauma still refers, for the
most part, to a physical impact, e.g., to the chest, head or elsewhere. There
are, for example, innumerable orthopaedic trauma centres.
What can cause a trauma?
An accident
Being under fire in a war
The death of a loved one
Rape
Cruelty
The King’s Cross fire
Zebrugge disaster
The Blitz
Being evacuated
Sexual abuse
There is no end to it. Any sort of psychic damage will do,
and it need not be defined to a single distressing event The idea of trauma as a
single event features in the DSM-III criteria (American Psychiatric Association,
1994, pp. 424-29), but an event no longer captures all the meaning of the term.
For example, Masud Khan coined the term ‘cumulative trauma’ (1963) to
characterize the breakdown of the mother’s role of protective shield, with
long-term consequences for the infant.
You begin to see why I mention many mansions.
I turn now to psychoanalytic concepts of trauma. Since the
broad field of traumatology and its burgeoning activities, writings and centres
are largely behavioural, you might assume that the concept of trauma has little
place in psychoanalytic writings. You will find 120 articles on the
Psychoanalytic Electronic Publishing CD-ROM containing over 30,000 articles from
six main psychoanalytic journals and 3914 occurrences of the term in all those
articles. That seems to me to be surprisingly few.
But if you turn to Freud you will find a whole page of the
Index to the Standard Edition devoted to references in his writings to trauma,
while Laplanche and Pontalis’ dictionary treats trauma as a thoroughly general
term accounting for the aetiology of the neuroses, while traumatic neurosis is
later introduced by Freud for something very near our PTSD. You will also find a
clear and useful account of the history of Freud’s thoughts on trauma in an
essay by the editor in Caroline Garland’s collection, Understanding Trauma: A
Psychoanalytic Approach. In her own contribution she makes a point which makes
nonsense of conceptions of trauma which concentrate on the external impact. Her
point is that what makes it not possible to get over that impact is that its
psychical meaning homes in on early object relations. She refers to ‘adhesions
that develop individual’s early history, particularly when the trauma is felt to
provide confirmation of early phantasies’. She also stresses that these links
can be ‘hard to shift because of the damage done by the traumatic event to the
survivor’s capacity to symbolise’, leading to an impoverishment of understanding
and communication (Garland, 1998, p. 7). She and her co-authors give innumerable
case studies of this dynamic drawn from their work at the Unit for the Study of
Trauma and Its Aftermath at the Tavistock Clinic, which she directs.
I want now to say something philosophical about all this. By
this I mean how we are here slip-sliding around from the language of bodily
impacts to that of events and enduring – perhaps incapacitating – forms of
distress in the inner world. It would be easy to be satirical about this and
mock the rhetoric of a relatively new and certainly only recently fashionable
approach and set of concepts. Of course, some concepts are, indeed, more subtle
and resonant and suggestive and enabling than others, which is why we have
literature and pulp, poetry and doggerel. However, I want to point out that
there is no escape from physical language in referring to mental events.
The founder of the conceptual framework within which, for
better and worse, we think, René Descartes, bequeathed to us a dualism of mind
and body which remains our everyday metaphysics nearly four centuries after he
wrote his Discourse on Method in 1637. One important feature, and the point of
my remarks at the moment, is that he defined body as having extension and motion
and as being susceptible to mathematical handling. He defined mind – very
unhelpfully – as that which does not pertain to body. It has no language or
parameters of its own; we speak of it by analogy to physical phenomena. In
succeeding centuries psychology, psychiatry, psychoanalysis have all had to
frame concepts in both formal and everyday language which expresses our mental
life in various analogies drawn from physics, chemistry, biology and other
material sciences – mental elements, compounds, structures, forces, energies,
impacts, positions, stresses. We think in terms of mental space (hence the title
of my recent book: Young, 1994) and that which makes it more capacious and
congenial as contrasted with that which restricts and closes it up.
So, though we may smile at the fashion in military and
athletic terms in he trauma industry, let us not deceive ourselves into thinking
that we can magically escape from the philosophical box Descartes left us in and
somehow evade bodily language and impacts. We can perhaps formulate more moving
stories, more subtle narratives, but we cannot evade the reliance on bodily
language inherent in our world view. Nor, however, should we give in to the
thuggery which says that the bodily story is the whole story. The bottom line is
emotion, and the vicissitudes of our emotions constitute our deepest psychology.
I want to cast my vote for narrative accounts in which trauma
is a word about suffering which is uncontainable and eliminates the afflicted
person’s capacity to defend his or her inner objects from anxiety. To have a
traumatic experience is to have one’s defences overwhelmed, resulting in a
failure of containment and stark exposure to primary anxieties, listed by Freud
as birth trauma, castration anxiety, loss of the loved object, loss of the
object’s love and the nameless dread of annihilation. All are linked to loss of
what is essential to life, and they lead to a state of melancholy (see Garland,
1998, ch. 1). Trauma engenders helplessness.
In speaking in favour of narrative I am speaking against
concentrating on nosology (Young, 1999). I say this, because Allan Young’s
account seems to me a cautionary tale in which a dedicated band of opponents of
the inner world, generated an approach to psychopathology which placed
classification at the heart of psychiatry at the expense of resonating with the
human heart, its sufferings and its vicissitudes.
Both his title, The Harmony of Illusions, and his subtitle,
Inventing Post Traumatic Stress Disorder, forcibly draw our attention to the
historicity of disease categories and to their social construction. He tells us
with great eloquence how PTSD was the consequence of framing suffering by a
lobby with uncategorised symptoms, the Vietnam War veterans (A. Young, 1995, p.
5; Kulka et al.,1990), intersecting with an audacious coup by psychiatrists who
followed the theory of classification of Emil Kraepelin, who argued that
psychiatric diseases were natural kinds like physical objects and who ruthlessly
and successfully purged all mention of the unconscious, psychodynamics and the
inner world from the third edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s
official bible, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, a
title usually shortened to DSM-III, published in 1980. Their leader, Roger
Spitzer, was quite forthright about this. He wrote that he had assembled a team
of writers ’committed to diagnostic research and not to clinical practice… with
intellectual roots in St. Louis instead of Vienna, and… intellectual inspiration
derived from Kraepelin, not Freud’ (A. Young, 1995, p. 99). The eminent
psychoanalyst Franz Alexander described Kraepelin as a ‘rigid and sterile
codifier of disease categories’. He, like Freud before him, saw no common ground
between Kraepelin’s ‘antipsychological’ approach and a psychodynamic one (p.
96). Thus were drawn up the battle lines between the psychodynamic and the
biological approaches which Tanya M. Luhrmann, in her recent and baleful
anthropological study, In Two Minds: The Growing Disorder in American Psychiatry
(2000), has examined in the training of psychiatrists, with the biological
reductionists in the ascendant at the expense of relating to, understanding and
treating the inside of human mental suffering.
The editors of DSM-III and DSM-IV profess to be biological in
their orientation, but this is no guarantee that the diagnostic categories which
they accept are based in natural science, that they are what scientists
concerned with classification call ’natural kinds’. Indeed, many of the
classifications in DSM are clearly the result of lobbying by social groups. The
most famous of these is homosexuality, which was a diagnostic category in
earlier editions of the manual. As a result of agitation by gays and lesbians it
was removed from the manual in the 1970s. The diagnosis ‘borderline disorder’
has been the subject of much debate, and there is a volume of essays assessing
its suitability (Silver and Rosenbleuth, 1992). As you move toward the back of
the manual, descriptions of adjustment and personality disorders become more and
more familiar descriptions of the vicissitudes of troubled people, sometimes
very like ourselves. I have been in the habit of reading out the criteria for
one called ‘Self-defeating Personality Disorder’ to my students, and they react
very uncomfortably, as if they have suddenly found themselves caught in the net
of psychiatric diagnosis (DSM-III-R, 1987, pp. 373-74). They were particularly
nervous about diagnostic criterion number six: ‘fails to accomplish tasks
crucial to his or her personal objectives despite demonstrated ability to do so,
e.g., helps fellow students write papers, but is unable to write his or her own’
(p. 374). Women were considered to me more likely than men to suffer from this
condition. As a result of agitation by feminists, the diagnosis was dropped and
did not appear in the next edition of DSM (Shorter, 1997, p. 305).
As I said, the inclusion of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
was the result of agitation. One historian of psychiatry comments that
‘psychiatric diagnosis was up for grabs’ (ibid.). He quotes Wilbur Scott, a
student of the campaign to get PTSD into DSM: ‘”PTSD is in DSM-III because a
core of psychiatrists and veterans worked consciously and deliberately for years
to put it there. They ultimately succeeded because they were better-organized,
more politically active, and enjoyed more lucky breaks than their opposition”’
(Scott, 1990, quoted in Shorter, 1997, pp. 304-5). Of course, in gaining
official recognition for their distress and suffering as a medical/psychiatric
diagnosis, they also gained access to treatment facilities compensation and
other benefits bestowed by the state on war injured personnel. Please do not
misunderstand my point. It is not to diminish the psychological impact of the
Vietnam War on all concerned. Rather, I want to draw attention to the benefits
of medicalising it in the form of a recognised, diagnosable mental disorder.
I want to conclude - and to reiterate my theme of many
perspectives on trauma (a house of many mansions) - with an eloquent and moving
quotation from Allan Young’s introduction which falls squarely inside the
philosophical tradition advocated by Richard Rorty which claims that truth is
made, not found. PTSD, he claims, is not a natural kind. The generally accepted
picture of it is mistaken.
The disorder is not timeless, nor does it possess an
intrinsic unity. Rather, it is glued together by the practices, technologies,
and narratives with which it is diagnosed, studied, treated, and represented and
by the various interests, institutions, and moral arguments that mobilized these
efforts and resources. If, as I am claiming, PTSD is a historical product, does
this mean that it is not real? Is this the significance of my book’s title? On
the contrary, the reality of PTSD is confirmed empirically by its place in
people’s lives, by their experiences and convictions, and by the personal and
collective investments that have been made in it. My job as an ethnographer of
PTSD is not to deny its reality but to explain how it and its traumatic memory
have been made real., to describe the mechanisms through which these phenomena
penetrate people’s life worlds, acquire facticity, and shape the self-knowledge
of patients, clinicians, and researchers. It is not doubt about the reality of
PTSD that separates me from the psychiatric insider. It is our divergent ideas
about the origins of this reality and its universality (the fact that we now
find it in many places and times)’ (A. Young, 1995, pp. 5-6)
He concludes by saying, as I have, that the suffering and
pain of PTSD is real, but this does not make the facts attached to it true,
i.e., timeless. Questions about truth cannot, he argues, ‘be divorced from the
social, cognitive and technological conditions through which researchers come to
know their facts and the meaning of facticity’ (p. 10)
Trauma is a useful metaphorical term. We have need of a
many-chambered house of ideas of trauma but not, I suggest, a fortress or an
arsenal or to be preoccupied with nosology rather than narrative in telling
stories about human suffering and in helping people to learn to contain and work
though their distress.
Chairman’s Opening Remarks (revised), conference on ‘Thinking
about Trauma: Connecting Theory and Practice’, sponsored by University of
Sheffield Centre for Psychotherapeutic Studies and Nottinghamshire Healthcare
NHS Trust, at Sheffield University, 22 June 2001.
REFERENCES
(Place of publication is London unless otherwise specified.)
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Edition — Revised). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association (DSM-III-R).
______ (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (Fourth Edition). Washington: American Psychiatric Association
(DSM-IV).
Barker, Pat (1991) Regeneration. Viking; reprinted
Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1992.
______ (1993) The Eye in the Door. Viking; reprinted
Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1994.
______ (1995) The Ghost Road. Viking; reprinted
Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1996.
Garland, Caroline, ed. (1998) Understanding Trauma: A
Psychoanalytical Approach. Duckworth.
Harrison, Tom (2000) Bion, Rickman, Foulkes and the
Northfield Experiments: Advancing on a Different Front. Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
Khan, Masud (1963) ‘The Concept of Cumulative Trauma’,
Psychoanal. Stud. Child 18: 286-306.
Kulka, Richard A. et al. (1990) Trauma and the Vietnam War
Generation; Report of the Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans
Readjustment Study. N. Y.: Brunner/Mazel.
Luhrmann, Tanya M. (2000) Of Two Minds: The Growing Disorder
of American Psychiatry. N. Y.: Knopf.
Scott, Wilbur J. (1990), ‘PTSD in DSM-III: A case in the
Politics of Diagnosis and Disease’, Social Problems 37: 294-310.
Showalter, Elaine (1985) The Female Malady. N. Y.: Pantheon.
Silver, Daniel and Rosenbleuth, Michael (1992) Handbook of
Borderline Disorders. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Young, Allan (1995) The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Princeton University Press.
Young, Robert M. (1994) Mental Space. Process Press.
______ (1998) ’Psychopathology: Term and Concept’, Distance
Learning Unit, Psychoanalytic Studies, University of Sheffield.
______ (1998a) ‘Descriptive v Psychodynamic Concepts of
Psychopathology’, Distance Learning Unit, Psychoanalytic Studies University of
Sheffield.
______ (1999) ’Between Nosology and Narrative: Where Should
We Be?’, talk delivered to the Toronto Psychoanalytic Society.
My own writings, including those listed above, are available
at http://human-nature.com/rmyoung/papers/
Copyright: The Author
Address for correspondence:
26 Freegrove Road, London N7 9RQ
robert@rmy1.demon.co.uk
http://human-nature.com/